|
От
|
Begletz
|
|
К
|
Александр
|
|
Дата
|
12.07.2001 07:01:13
|
|
Рубрики
|
Глобализация;
|
|
Спасибо. Ну, как я и предполагал...
Аргументы статьи построены не на объективных данных, а на результатах опроса. А тут главна дело-задать правильно вопрос. Читаем: A child was classified as food-insufficient if the respondent to the family questionnaire reported
>that the family either sometimes or often did not get enough food to eat.
Гениально! Иногда или часто! Почему меня не спросили, гады? Я бы тоже честно ответил "Да!" (Dah!) Потому что приехали мы в Ямерику с 50 баксами и двумя проглотами, которые непрерывно просили жрать, и 1й месяц, бывало, говорили им вечерком: все, пошли спать нафиг, если счас вас кормить, то останетесь без завтрака. Но ведь было, значит-sometimes! А ты меня ещще в "Купленную Елиту" записал...Нехорошо бедных-то обижать, Сашок! Короче, на такой вопрос почти любой лоу-класс америкос ответит "да". Тем более, что значит enough food to eat? Два куска пиццы съел, а 3го нету? Далее, делаем повышенную выборку черных и мексиканцев, и показываем, что их дети плохо учатся. Что скорее всего правда. Хотя то ли они бедные, оттого что глупые, то ли глупые, оттого что бедные, ещще неизвестно. При этом скромно умолчим, что каждый 9й черный в Америке зарегистрирован, как наркоман, поэтому имеет повышенные шансы нарожать дурковатых деток. А ведь есть объективные методики, которые позволяют, не задавая хитрых вопросов, измерить вес, %% жировой ткани и пр. показатели того, голодает человек, или нет. Потому что ежели человека не кормить, он, представь себе, худеет. Короче, Александр, это туфта.
>Ну нате Вам
>Institution: HOUSTON ACADEMY OF MED || Sign In as Member/Non-Member (Individual) ||
>Contact Subscription Administrator at your institution || FAQ
>PEDIATRICS Vol. 108 No. 1 July 2001, pp. 44-53
>Food Insufficiency and American School-Aged
>Children's Cognitive, Academic, and
>Psychosocial Development
>Katherine Alaimo, PhD*, Christine M. Olson, PhD, RD, and Edward A.
>Frongillo Jr, PhD
>From the * School of Public Health, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor,
>Michigan; and the Division of Nutritional Sciences, Cornell University, Ithaca,
>New York.
> ABSTRACT
>Objective. This study investigates associations between food insufficiency and cognitive, academic, and
>psychosocial outcomes for US children and teenagers ages 6 to 11 and 12 to 16 years.
>Methods. Data from the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES III) were
>analyzed. Children were classified as food-insufficient if the family respondent reported that his or her family
>sometimes or often did not get enough food to eat. Regression analyses were conducted to test for associations between food
>insufficiency and cognitive, academic, and psychosocial measures in general and then within lower-risk and higher-risk groups.
>Regression coefficients and odds ratios for food insufficiency are reported, adjusted for poverty status and other potential
>confounding factors.
>Results. After adjusting for confounding variables, 6- to 11-year-old food-insufficient children had significantly lower arithmetic
>scores and were more likely to have repeated a grade, have seen a psychologist, and have had difficulty getting along with other
>children. Food-insufficient teenagers were more likely to have seen a psychologist, have been suspended from school, and have
>had difficulty getting along with other children. Further analyses divided children into lower-risk and higher-risk groups. The
>associations between food insufficiency and children's outcomes varied by level of risk.
>Conclusions. The results demonstrate that negative academic and psychosocial outcomes are associated with family-level
>food insufficiency and provide support for public health efforts to increase the food security of American families. Key words:
>hunger, food insecurity, food insufficiency, poverty, psychosocial development, cognition, children, NHANES III.
>Although they live in a wealthy nation, children in the United States are not immune to poverty and hunger. Almost 1 in 5
>American children are poor, and >14 million children under age 18 live in food-insecure households, in which there is a limited
>or uncertain availability of nutritionally adequate or safe foods.1,2 Although these numbers demonstrate the magnitude of this
>social problem, they do not adequately express the real burden of food deprivation for American children.
>Research on child development in nonindustrialized countries shows that malnutrition is associated with delays in motor skills,
>cognitive deficits, and decreases in school performance.3-6 Current theory postulates that malnutrition's effect occurs through
>motivational and emotional behaviors rather than influencing intelligence directly. Severely malnourished children have been
>shown to be apathetic, withdrawn, and passive and have decreased motivation and heightened anxiety.3,4
>Questions remain about whether the level of food deprivation in the United States is severe enough to affect children's cognitive
>and psychosocial outcomes. Furthermore, most studies on this topic have been conducted with children younger than school
>age. Recently, in 2 studies that used data from the Community Childhood Hunger Identification Project surveys, investigators
>found that school-aged children identified as hungry or at risk of hunger were more likely to have impaired psychosocial function
>and to have received psychological counseling, were more likely to be absent from or late to school, were more likely to be
>receiving special education services, and were marginally more likely to have repeated a grade.7,8
>In this article, we examine the relationships between family food insufficiency and cognitive, academic, and psychosocial
>outcomes in a nationally representative sample of American school-aged children.
> METHODS
> NHANES III Data
>Data for children ages 6 to 11 years (n = 3286) and 12 to 16 years (n = 2063) were analyzed from the
>Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES III), a cross-sectional representative
>sample of the US civilian noninstitutionalized population living in households (homeless people were not
>included). The survey was conducted from 1988 to 1994. Mexican Americans and black Americans were oversampled to
>provide more reliable estimates for these groups. Detailed descriptions of the sample design and operation of the survey have
>been published elsewhere.9
>NHANES III included medical and cognitive examinations and interviews conducted with survey participants and proxy
>respondents. For this analysis, we used data from the Household Family and Household Youth Questionnaires (proxy
>interviews) conducted in the home, Youth and Proxy Questionnaires conducted in the mobile examination centers, and blood
>samples collected in the mobile examination centers. Of the proxy respondents, 89% were mothers and 6% were fathers of the
>child; the rest were other relatives or caregivers familiar with the child. All interviews were administered using standard
>protocols by trained interviewers.9
> Theoretical Model
>Using past research, a theoretical model of the ecological factors affecting school-aged children's cognitive, academic, and
>psychosocial development was created using variables available from NHANES III.10-21 We postulated that poor cognitive,
>academic, and psychosocial outcomes are caused by deficiencies in family and child resources, environmental risks, past
>nutrition, health, and social risks, and family food insufficiency. The variables used to test this model and their age group
>availability in NHANES III are shown in Table 1.
> View this table:
> [in this window]
> [in a new window]
> TABLE 1
> NHANES III Analyses Variables and Age Groups
>Food insufficiency has been previously shown to be negatively associated with children's health status after adjusting for these
>other factors.22 In addition to postulating that food insufficiency is associated with poor cognitive, academic, and psychosocial
>outcomes, we hypothesized that food insufficiency affects children's outcomes through their health status.
> Cognitive, Academic, and Psychosocial Outcomes
>Cognitive functioning was assessed using 2 subtests of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R): Block
>Design, a perceptual organization examination in which children are asked to construct designs out of blocks to match a model;
>and Digit Span, a freedom-from-distractibility examination in which children are asked to repeat up to 8 digits in forward and
>reverse directions.23,24 Academic scores were assessed using 2 subtests of the Wide Range Achievement Test-Revised
>(WRAT-R): Reading and Arithmetic.25 The scores for all 4 subtests were standardized to a common scale of 0 to 20 for each
>age based on samples obtained by the test developers.10,23,25 The full WISC-R and WRAT-R have been used extensively to
>assess children's IQ and academic skills. Because only portions of these tests were administered during NHANES III, the
>subtests cannot be used to calculate an IQ score or an achievement score.
>Psychosocial outcomes included the number of days the child had been absent from school in the previous year, whether the
>child had ever seen a psychiatrist, psychologist, or psychoanalyst for any emotional, mental, or behavioral problems, whether
>the child had ever been suspended from school, the number of good friends the child had, whether the child had difficulty getting
>along with other children or teenagers, and whether the child was somewhat shy and slow to make a new friend.
> Sociodemographic Data and Family and Child Resources
>For each child in the survey, information about sex, age, race-ethnicity, health insurance status, family size, number of rooms in
>the family residence, number of times the family had moved, metropolitan or nonmetropolitan region of residence, family
>income, whether they had a regular source of health care, and employment status and education of the family head were
>provided by a responsible adult living in the home. The family head was a person who owned or rented the home where the
>child lived.
>Total family income for the previous 12 months was reported for categories ranging from <$1000 to $80 000 and over, in
>$1000 increments below $19 999, in $5000 increments between $20 000 and $49 999, and in $10 000 increments between
>$50 000 and $79 999. A poverty index ratio (PIR) was then calculated by comparing the midpoint of the category and the
>child's family size with the federal poverty line.26 These analyses used 3 poverty status categories: low income (PIR 130% of
>the poverty line, which is the federal cutoff point for eligibility for the Food Stamp Program), middle income (>130% to 350%
>of the poverty line), and high income (>350% of the poverty line). A child was defined as insured if she or he was covered
>during the last month by private health insurance, military health care insurance, or Medicaid and if the coverage paid for more
>than accidents.
>Twelve percent of the WISC and WRAT subtests were conducted in Spanish; therefore, for these analyses we controlled for
>the language in which the test was conducted. For analyses of WRAT and WISC scores, information on race, ethnicity, and the
>language used during the proxy interview was used to classify children into 4 race-ethnic categories: all non-Hispanic white
>children and other children with cognitive tests conducted in English, all non-Hispanic black children, Mexican-American
>children with cognitive tests conducted in English, and Mexican-American or other children with cognitive tests conducted in
>Spanish. For analyses of other outcomes, we controlled for language by creating the same categories with the main language the
>child spoke at home.
> Food Insufficiency
>For the purpose of the NHANES III survey, food insufficiency was defined as "an inadequate amount of food intake due to a
>lack of money or resources." A child was classified as food-insufficient if the respondent to the family questionnaire reported
>that the family either sometimes or often did not get enough food to eat. This question has undergone cognitive testing1927-31 and
>has been demonstrated to be associated with food expenditure and nutrient intake.32-34
> Environmental Risk
>For all children, the child's blood lead concentration was measured (in micrograms per deciliter). Family size was divided by the
>number of rooms in the family residence to determine a continuous measure of crowded housing status.
> Past Health, Nutrition, and Social Risk
>For children 6 to 11 years, information was collected on the mother's age at birth, the presence of birth complications, low birth
>weight (birth weight below 2500 g), and any prenatal smoke exposure. A child was classified as having attended childcare if he
>or she had ever attended childcare where there were 6 or more children before he or she was 4 years old. We used height (in
>meters) measured at the time of the examination as an indicator of past nutrition status.
> Health Status
>Proxy-reported health status provides a general summary of children's health. Proxy respondents for the household youth
>questionnaire were asked to describe the child's health as excellent, very good, good, fair, or poor. For this analysis, the fair
>and poor categories were combined because <1% of children were reported to be in poor health.
> Statistical Methods
>Data for children 6 to 11 years of age and teenagers 12 to 16 years of age were analyzed separately. Using the theoretical
>model as a guide, the analyses were conducted in 4 stages. First, main effect linear regression models (for continuous
>outcomes) or logistic (for dichotomous outcomes) or ordinal logistic (for ordinal outcomes) regression models were created to
>test the hypothesis that food insufficiency is associated with cognitive, academic, or psychosocial outcomes, independent of
>other potential confounders. All variables in the theoretical model were included in the regression models and included the
>variables shown in Table 1. Food insufficiency has been shown to be associated with health status.22 In the second stage of the
>analyses, health status was added to the models to determine whether the effect of food insufficiency on the outcomes could
>have acted through health status.
>The third stage of the analyses created a risk factor index. Previous research has shown that risk factors affect children's
>cognitive, academic, and psychosocial performance in an additive fashion.2035-39 We used this premise to test for interactions
>between food insufficiency and other risk factors. We created a risk factor index by summing up the negative risk variables
>following the approach of Sameroff et al.35-37 Variables chosen for the risk indices for each age category were based on their
>availability in the NHANES III survey. For 6- to 11-year-old children, the following were given a value of 1 each and then
>summed together: an unemployed family head, an unmarried family head, a crowded dwelling (>1 person per room), moved
>3 or more times in his or her lifetime, no health insurance, no regular source of health care, blood lead exposure (10 µg/dL),
>birth weight below 2500 g, exposure to prenatal smoke, birth complications, a mother under age 18 at child's birth, and no child
>care attendance. For teenagers, the following were given a value of 1 each and then summed together: an unemployed family
>head, an unmarried family head, a crowded dwelling (>1 person per room), moved 3 or more times, no health insurance, no
>regular source of health care, blood lead exposure, and no child care attendance.
>The fourth stage of the analyses looked for interactive associations. The risk indices allowed the children to be divided into
>2 categories: lower risk (0-2 risks) and higher risk (3 or more risks). Regression models were created to assess the relationship
>between food insufficiency and the outcomes within each risk category. In these models, PIR, education of the family head,
>height, and WISC scores (for academic outcomes) were entered as continuous control variables and sex, age, metropolitan
>region, and race-ethnicity as categorical control variables. Interactions between food insufficiency and PIR, race-ethnicity, and
>education of the family head were also assessed and found not to be meaningfully significant.
>Sample weights were created for the NHANES III data to account for the oversampling of certain groups, such as black
>Americans and Mexican Americans, as well as nonresponse. For all analyses, NHANES III weighted data were analyzed using
>the svy commands available in Stata Statistical Software.40 These commands use the weights and survey cluster design to
>calculate accurate point estimates and variances. Investigation of outliers and influential data points revealed that unusual
>children with high sample weights had strong influence on the analyses, which reduced the accuracy of our estimates. To
>address this problem, we transformed the sample weights using the square root to reduce somewhat the skew of the sample
>weights.41 This kept the integrity of the relationship among sampled children to the total population while reducing the influence
>of a few individual children.
>For prevalence estimates and means, missing data were excluded from the analyses. For the regression analyses, all missing
>data except food insufficiency status were imputed using the impute command in STATA, which uses regression equations to
>fill in missing values based on other nonmissing data in the child's record. Variables included in these regression equations were
>chosen separately for each imputed variable using backward stepwise regression to screen for associated variables. For
>dichotomous variables, impute was used to predict a probability, and a random value was selected based on this probability.
>The number of missing values imputed ranged from 0 children missing data for whether the child had a regular source of health
>care to 255 6- to 11-year-old children and 176 teenagers missing data for their family's poverty index ratio. A total of
>336 6- to 11-year-old children and 450 teenagers had at least 1 missing value.
> RESULTS
> Unadjusted Means and Prevalence Estimates
>Unadjusted mean scores and percentages for cognitive, academic, and psychosocial outcomes are shown for
>food-insufficient and food-sufficient children in Table 2. Mean scores of the WISC and WRAT subtests by
>demographic characteristics have previously been published.10 For both younger children and teenagers,
>WRAT and WISC scores were approximately 1.3 to 2.5 points lower (out of a scale of 20) for food-insufficient children than
>for food-sufficient children. In addition, food-insufficient children and teenagers were more than twice as likely to have repeated
>a grade and missed more school days. For example, >40% of food-insufficient teenagers had repeated a grade, as compared
>with 20.7% of food-sufficient teenagers.
> View this table:
> [in this window]
> [in a new window]
> TABLE 2
> Unadjusted Means and Prevalences of Cognitive, Academic, and Psychosocial Outcomes by
> Food Sufficiency Status for Children and Teenagers: NHANES III, 1988-1994
>Food-insufficient children and teenagers were also more likely to have psychosocial difficulties than those who were
>food-sufficient. For teenagers, these differences were dramatic: Food-insufficient teenagers were more than twice as likely to
>have seen a psychologist, almost 3 times as likely to have been suspended, almost twice as likely to have a lot or some difficulty
>getting along with others, and 4 times as likely to have no friends.
> Regression Analyses
>Results of the main effects regression analyses for the relationships between food insufficiency and the outcomes controlling for
>other characteristics are shown in Table 3. Coefficients and odds ratios for other variables in the models are shown in Appendix
>Tables 1-4. The main effect models showed that after adjusting for potential confounding variables, for 6- to 11-year-old
>children food insufficiency was significantly negatively associated with WRAT arithmetic scores and positively associated with
>having repeated a grade and seen a psychologist. Arithmetic scores were .40 points lower for food-insufficient children than for
>food-sufficient children. Food-insufficient children were 1.44 times more likely to have repeated a grade and 1.89 times more
>likely to have seen a psychologist. Food insufficiency was not significantly associated with cognitive outcomes, reading scores,
>and other psychosocial outcomes.
> View this table:
> [in this window]
> [in a new window]
> TABLE 3
> Coefficients and Odds Ratios for Food Insufficiency: NHANES III, 1988-1994
>Food-insufficient teenagers were almost twice as likely to have seen a psychologist, have been suspended from school, and
>have difficulty getting along with other children as food-sufficient teenagers. Food insufficiency was not significantly related to
>any of the cognitive or academic outcomes.
>The effect of food insufficiency for these outcomes was only slightly attenuated by the addition of health status in the model,
>which suggests that if the association between food insufficiency and the outcomes was causal, the effect of food insufficiency on
>these outcomes did not act primarily through diminished health status (see Appendix Tables 1-4).
> Risk Factor Indices
>Food insufficiency is not the only risk factor many American children face. The number of risk factors a child had was plotted
>against his or her academic, cognitive, and psychosocial outcomes (Fig 1 as an example). With increasing numbers of risk
>factors, 6- to 11-year-old children's WISC and WRAT scores decreased (Fig 1), and their odds of negative academic and
>psychosocial outcomes increased (data not shown). For teenagers, WISC and WRAT scores also decreased with increasing
>number of risk factors, but there was no clear relationship between number of risk factors and psychosocial outcomes (data not
>shown).
> View larger version (19K):
> [in this window]
> [in a new window]
> Fig. 1. Relationship between number of risk factors and cognitive and
> academic outcomes: 6- to 11-year-old children
> Stratified Analyses
>The risk indices were used to divide children and teenagers into 2 groups: lower risk (0-2 risks) and higher risk (3 or more
>risks). Table 4 shows the coefficients and odds ratios by food insufficiency status for cognitive, academic, and psychosocial
>outcomes in the 2 groups. In the younger children, both arithmetic and reading scores were lower in food-insufficient children in
>the lower-risk group but not in the higher-risk group. Food-insufficient children were more likely to have repeated a grade in
>both the lower- and higher-risk groups, but the odds ratio was larger in the lower-risk group (2.12 vs 1.41). Food insufficiency
>was not related to block or digit scores or to number of school days lost in either the lower- or higher-risk groups. Higher-risk
>food-insufficient children were more likely to have seen a psychologist, but the odds ratios for these outcomes were similar in
>the lower-risk group. Higher-risk 6- to 11-year-old children were also more likely to have difficulty getting along with other
>children (P < .10).
> View this table:
> [in this window]
> [in a new window]
> TABLE 4
> Coefficients and Odds Ratios for Food Insufficiency by Lower- and Higher-Risk Groups
>For the teenagers, food-insufficient children did not have significantly lower WRAT or WISC scores in either the lower- or
>higher-risk groups, except for lower digit scores in the higher-risk group (.50; P < .10). However, lower-risk
>food-insufficient teenagers were more likely to have problems in all psychosocial outcomes except shyness. Both lower- and
>higher-risk groups of food-insufficient teenagers were more likely to have been suspended from school, but the odds ratio for
>the lower-risk food-insufficient children was higher than for the higher-risk food-insufficient children (2.40 vs 1.87).
> DISCUSSION
>The results of this study demonstrate that family food insufficiency is associated with school-aged children's
>academic and psychosocial development. They support the growing body of research on the negative
>consequences of food insecurity and hunger for American children. Studies that have evaluated the effects of
>US nutrition programs, such as the Special Supplemental Food Program for Women, Infants and Children,
>Head Start, and the School Lunch and Breakfast Programs,42-45 have shown small but significant benefits of
>food supplementation in cognition, academic achievement, and school absence. In addition, a positive relationship between
>some measure of nutritional status (such as dietary intake or height) and cognition or achievement has been found in most
>cross-sectional studies conducted in the United States.1546-52 Most recently, in 2 studies that used data from the Community
>Childhood Hunger Identification Project surveys, investigators found significant associations between being hungry or at risk of
>hunger and psychosocial and academic outcomes.7,8
>The results reported here are important because they are the first to demonstrate an association between a questionnaire-based
>measure of family food insufficiency and negative developmental outcomes in a national sample of school-aged children.
>Furthermore, these results demonstrate that food insufficiency is associated with negative outcomes even after adjusting for
>many other factors that are known to place children's development at risk. Questionnaire-based measures of food insufficiency,
>food insecurity, or hunger are more appropriate than anthropometric measures for recognizing a relationship between current
>resource-constrained food deprivation and negative outcomes because they capture the phenomena of interest directly rather
>than through indirect indicators. For example, an indicator that has been used in previous research, child's height, is a better
>measure of past rather than current nutrition deprivation and is significantly influenced by other factors such as genetics. The
>NHANES III food insufficiency questionnaire was a precursor to the new Food Insecurity and Hunger scale developed in the
>mid-1990s by the US Department of Agriculture in collaboration with other federal agencies, advocacy groups, and university
>researchers. The Food Insecurity and Hunger questionnaire has been fielded annually since 1995 as part of the Current
>Population Survey. It is hoped that future research on the outcomes of food insecurity and hunger in the United States will use
>all or a portion of this questionnaire.1,53
>Research on children's cognitive and psychosocial development suggests that developmental problems do not have single
>causes, nor do risks have specific outcomes, but rather that risk factors are additive and the more risks a child has, the worse
>the outcomes of all types.2035-39,54 Therefore, specificity of risk factors is less important than the total number of risks present.
>Our results support this hypothesis by demonstrating that each additional risk increases the likelihood that a child will have
>poorer outcomes. Studies of this nature have not typically included poor nutrition or food deprivation as risk variables. The
>results of our study illustrate that family food insufficiency is another risk factor that should be considered.
>The regression analyses stratified by risk level revealed associations not previously detected, such as the association between
>food insufficiency and lower reading scores in children with fewer other risk factors. Furthermore, these analyses revealed that
>for some (but not all) academic and psychosocial outcomes, food insufficiency was more strongly associated with poorer
>outcomes in children with fewer other risk factors. The implications of these findings are not that it is unimportant to ensure that
>higher-risk children receive enough food but rather that food insufficiency may not be the only challenge some children face;
>ensuring food sufficiency without addressing other risk factors may not lead to measurable improvements. On the other hand,
>for children and teenagers with fewer other risk factors, providing their families with food alone may improve some academic
>and psychosocial outcomes.
>Several limitations are associated with using the NHANES III survey. First, although the richness of NHANES III allowed us
>to delineate the relationship between food insufficiency and academic and psychosocial outcomes after adjusting for many other
>known risk factors, there are several important risk factors that we could not include in our analyses. For example, duration of
>poverty, school and neighborhood influences, personality traits of the child, and parental characteristics have been demonstrated
>to be associated with children's development.14,55 In addition, because the NHANES III is a cross-sectional survey, it is not
>possible to determine how the various factors we identified to be associated with poor outcomes in children interacted in causal
>sequences. Finally, the NHANES III sample excluded homeless children and thereby excluded a significant portion of
>food-insufficient children in the United States.
>The negative outcomes associated with family food insufficiency can be serious both for children as they are experiencing
>problems and for the adults they will grow to be. Parker and Asher56 have found that having problems getting along with other
>children is a risk factor for later difficulties such as criminality and dropping out of school. In addition, low academic
>achievement in younger ages and grade failure is a predictor of low long-term education achievement and later behavior
>problems.57
>Given that food insufficiency is associated with children's academic and psychosocial development, what are the potential
>mechanisms for this process? Although family food insufficiency has previously been associated with poorer